THE HUMAN VOICE AS A NEGLECTED PSYCHO-SOCIAL RESEARCH TOPIC AND RESOURCE 1
There is an excellent
fit between the speaking voice and the psycho-social approach, in that the
voice connects inner and outer worlds while simultaneously challenging such a
division. It remains, however, relatively neglected, both as a psycho-social
research resource and as a topic for the psycho-social researcher. This article
argues that, while researchers are developing increasingly sophisticated ways
of harnessing visual research methods, the oral dimension remains marginalised,
with voice almost invariably collapsed into speech. Despite the methodological
challenges created by using the voice as a psycho-social research tool,
attention to the paralinguistic has the potential to enrich research and deepen
our psycho-social understanding of human behaviour. The speaking voice is the
nodal point of human communication: notwithstanding the growth of social media
and virtual bonds, it remains the prime instrument through which we establish
relations with others, acting as connective tissue among both strangers and
intimates. Given this centrality, the neglect of the speaking voice as a
psycho-social research topic and resource is curious, especially in the context
of the enormous interest in conversation and speech over the past few decades.
Yet where voice is referred to at all it is often its metaphorical meanings
that are signalled: the voice as signifier for political presence and power, a
synonym for enfranchisement. Literary studies have also appropriated it, along
with other terms from the auditory lexicon such as tone and register, to signal
narrative viewpoint or perspective. Voice 1 This article is based on a paper
given at the Association of Psycho-Social Research conference at the University
of West of England, Bristol, June 2016, and Karpf ‘s PhD submission, The persistence
of the oral: on the enduring importance of the human voice (2016) 2 Contact:
A.Karpf@londonmet.ac.uk Anne Karp Listen! The human voice as a neglected
psycho-social research topic and resource 34 is often also elided with dialect
or accent, especially in the UK, or it is used as a proxy for speech and
language. Again and again major thinkers sidle up to the voice, seem about to
grasp it and then turn on their heels. Bourdieu (2014), for example, came
tantalisingly close to describing how social and cultural factors are
‘implicated’ in the voice but never quite spelt it out. Yet his notion of
‘doxa’, the relationship between habitus and ‘the field to which it is
attuned’, conceives of the body as a ‘living memory pad’ (Bourdieu, 2014,
p.68), producing ‘body automatisms’ that are below the level of consciousness,
including bodily expressions of emotion such as laughter, along with
‘deep-rooted linguistic and muscular patterns of behaviour’ (Bourdieu, 2014,
p.69). The human voice is located precisely at the nexus of the linguistic and
the muscular. Or there is Ahmed (2004), who makes many illuminating points
about signs and how they work on and in relation to bodies through their
‘stickiness’. Hate speech becomes sticky, she argues, through repetition, e.g.
of the word ‘Paki’, which accumulates a history to which it is bound. She
suggests, further, that naming something as disgusting in a speech act is
performative; thereby, the sign itself becomes ‘sticky’ and fetishistic. Yet,
remarkably, the fact that we learn to recognise these signs in part through
intonation is relegated to a footnote, even though it is surely the oral
register through which it is shouted in hate speech that is the most ‘sticky’
aspect of the word ‘Paki’, and disgust is performed at least as much through
the vocalisations that accompany words such as ‘That’s disgusting!’ or, more
succinctly, ‘Yuk!’ Strangest of all, Wetherell (2012), in her pioneering
exploration of the circulation of affect, alights on vocal states in some of
her case-histories, especially in her analysis of the conflict between a pair
of teenage girls, but never dwells on their vocality. She argues for the
multimodality of situated affective practices, including bodily actions and
storytelling, that together create ‘an integrated Journal of Psychosocial
Studies, Volume 11, Issue 2, October 2018 35 and organic unfolding and weaving’
(p.89). Is there not space here for the modality of voice? The purr, the gabble
and the whine, are they not constitutive of meaning, a symphony of signs that
draw on cultural repertoires of shared understandings that they also help
construct? This article suggests that the speaking voice is a rich research
topic for psycho-social researchers. It is also a potentially valuable research
tool, even though as a resource too it has been neglected. Over the past
decades visual methods, from photographs and documentary film to video diaries,
have been increasingly embraced by qualitative researchers and yet attention to
the oral and aural has scarcely developed. An innovative research method, the
‘visual matrix’, currently being developed, for instance, explores ways of
using ‘imagery, affect and visualisation… to inquire into phenomena that
research participants may find difficult to put into words’ (Visual Matrix
Workshop, 2015). This has been deployed interestingly to help gauge reaction to
local public art (Froggett, 2014). Yet although the final report of the project
refers to the ‘emotional tone, pace and vibrancy’ (p.61) of participants’ reactions,
it is (understandably perhaps, given its remit) mostly mute about the vocal
dimension. Plummer (2001) devotes three lines to sound archives in his
catalogue of ‘documents for life’ compared with a page-and-a-half to visual
data. Reasons for the marginalisation of the voice A number of different
factors are responsible for the discursive absence of the speaking voice. While
ideas around embodiment have provided a supportive milieu in which to develop
thinking about the voice, they do not provide a perfect fit. Just as the
concept of ‘nonverbal behaviour’ has proved to be a valuable conceptual home
for research into the voice yet also one with limitations (since, pace
Bourdieu, and leaving aside bodily sounds such as sighs and sobs, Anne Karp
Listen! The human voice as a neglected psycho-social research topic and
resource 36 grunts and cries, it is hard to conceive of voice without words),
so too does embodiment take us only so far. For the paradox faced by both
researchers, interviewees and indeed speakers themselves is that the voice is
the product of the solid materiality of the body and yet, as soon as it has
been produced, becomes fleeting, temporary and insubstantial ‘materiality at
its most intangible’ (Dolar, 2006, p.59). ‘That which is only audible’, wrote
Simmel, an early theorist of the senses, ‘is already past in the moment of its
present and provides no ‘property’’ (Simmel, 1907). The voice, as Dolar puts
it, ‘is like a bodily missile which separates itself from the body…. the voice
is plus-de-corps: both the surplus of the body, a bodily excess, and the
nomore-body, the end of the corporeal’ (Dolar, 2006, pp. 70-71). In addition,
the increasing dissemination of voice through electronic media, along with the
emergence of the synthetic voice, problematises the identification of the voice
with the body. The auditory field, at least in modern Western cultures, is thus
harder to analyse than the visual field and gesture has proved easier to
describe than voice. (Interestingly, spatial metaphors are often deployed to
elucidate the paradoxical nature of the voice. As with Dolar, above, so LaBelle
conceptualises the voice ‘as something expelled from the mouth, but which never
leaves me behind… The voice does not move away from my body the voice stretches
me; it drags me along’ (LaBelle, 2014, p.5.) This may help explain the
difficulty of researchers in retaining a focus on the audible voice. Indeed, a
great deal of the interest in the embodied voice arose just at the point where,
thanks to new communications technologies, it was becoming disembodied. Perhaps
we should be looking elsewhere for a sensitivity to vocal communication, for
instance in the proliferation since the early 1990s of scholarly literature in
the field of sound studies. 3 Which have recently acquired their own
biographers, e.g Connor, 2014; LaBelle, 2014; Gillie, 2010 Journal of
Psychosocial Studies, Volume 11, Issue 2, October 2018 37 One of the earliest
and most influential texts was Murray Schafer’s exploration of the
‘soundscape’, an elegy for and a celebration of a lost, rich sonic world, first
published in 1977. Murray Schafer argued that the soundscape was changing, with
humans now inhabiting an acoustic environment different from any known before
and one in which they suffered from the effects of noise pollution (Murray
Schafer, 1994). In response Schafer originated the concept of ‘acoustic
ecology’, a deep appreciation and understanding of the relationship between
human, animals and the sonic environment (Cummings, 2001). The studies that
followed explored sonic culture in all its variety, amounting to ‘a cultural
phenomenology of mediated aural practices’ (Droumeva and Andrisani, 2011).
Coming at the same time as a boom in interest in both oral and aural cultural
forms from the audio book and podcast to the soundwalk and sound art it might
be surmised that we are living in a golden age of interest in sound. It is
striking, however, that the human speaking voice occupies a relatively
insignificant position in much of the research in sound studies and mediated
aurality.
A Research proposal for the human voice as a neglected psycho-social research topic and resource 1:
Reviews: A Review on the human voice as a neglected psycho-social research topic and resource 1, human, voice, neglected project topics, researchcub.info, project topic, list of project topics, research project topics, journals, books, Academic writer.
There is an excellent fit between the speaking voice and the psycho-social approach, in that the voice connects inner and outer worlds while simultaneously challenging such a division. It remains, however, relatively neglected, both as a psycho-social research resource and as a topic for the psycho-social researcher. This article argues that, while researchers are developing increasingly sophisticated ways of harnessing visual research methods, the oral dimension remains marginalised, with voice almost invariably collapsed into speech. Despite the methodological challenges created by using the voice as a psycho-social research tool, attention to the paralinguistic has the potential to enrich research and deepen our psycho-social understanding of human behaviour. The speaking voice is the nodal point of human communication: notwithstanding the growth of social media and virtual bonds, it remains the prime instrument through which we establish relations with others, acting as connective tissue among both strangers and intimates. Given this centrality, the neglect of the speaking voice as a psycho-social research topic and resource is curious, especially in the context of the enormous interest in conversation and speech over the past few decades. Yet where voice is referred to at all it is often its metaphorical meanings that are signalled: the voice as signifier for political presence and power, a synonym for enfranchisement. Literary studies have also appropriated it, along wi.. animal science project topics
THE HUMAN VOICE AS A NEGLECTED PSYCHO-SOCIAL RESEARCH TOPIC AND RESOURCE 1